Papers
Topics
Authors
Recent
Detailed Answer
Quick Answer
Concise responses based on abstracts only
Detailed Answer
Well-researched responses based on abstracts and relevant paper content.
Custom Instructions Pro
Preferences or requirements that you'd like Emergent Mind to consider when generating responses
Gemini 2.5 Flash
Gemini 2.5 Flash 37 tok/s
Gemini 2.5 Pro 41 tok/s Pro
GPT-5 Medium 10 tok/s Pro
GPT-5 High 15 tok/s Pro
GPT-4o 84 tok/s Pro
Kimi K2 198 tok/s Pro
GPT OSS 120B 448 tok/s Pro
Claude Sonnet 4 31 tok/s Pro
2000 character limit reached

LLM Theory of Mind and Alignment: Opportunities and Risks (2405.08154v1)

Published 13 May 2024 in cs.HC and cs.AI

Abstract: LLMs are transforming human-computer interaction and conceptions of AI with their impressive capacities for conversing and reasoning in natural language. There is growing interest in whether LLMs have theory of mind (ToM); the ability to reason about the mental and emotional states of others that is core to human social intelligence. As LLMs are integrated into the fabric of our personal, professional and social lives and given greater agency to make decisions with real-world consequences, there is a critical need to understand how they can be aligned with human values. ToM seems to be a promising direction of inquiry in this regard. Following the literature on the role and impacts of human ToM, this paper identifies key areas in which LLM ToM will show up in human:LLM interactions at individual and group levels, and what opportunities and risks for alignment are raised in each. On the individual level, the paper considers how LLM ToM might manifest in goal specification, conversational adaptation, empathy and anthropomorphism. On the group level, it considers how LLM ToM might facilitate collective alignment, cooperation or competition, and moral judgement-making. The paper lays out a broad spectrum of potential implications and suggests the most pressing areas for future research.

Definition Search Book Streamline Icon: https://streamlinehq.com
References (67)
  1. Merlyn mind, 2023.
  2. Replika, 2024.
  3. Woebot health, 2024.
  4. Gpt-4 technical report. arXiv preprint arXiv:2303.08774 (2023).
  5. Anthropic. Claude’s constitution.
  6. A general language assistant as a laboratory for alignment. arXiv preprint arXiv:2112.00861 (2021).
  7. Training a helpful and harmless assistant with reinforcement learning from human feedback. arXiv preprint arXiv:2204.05862 (2022).
  8. Constitutional ai: Harmlessness from ai feedback. arXiv preprint arXiv:2212.08073 (2022).
  9. The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 42, 2 (2001), 241–251.
  10. On the opportunities and risks of foundation models. arXiv preprint arXiv:2108.07258 (2021).
  11. Language models are few-shot learners. Advances in neural information processing systems 33 (2020), 1877–1901.
  12. Sparks of artificial general intelligence: Early experiments with gpt-4. arXiv preprint arXiv:2303.12712 (2023).
  13. Intelligent assistants have poor usability: A user study of alexa, google assistant, and siri, July 2018.
  14. Why anthropomorphize? folk psychology and other stories. Anthropomorphism, anecdotes, and animals (1997), 59–73.
  15. Deep reinforcement learning from human preferences. Advances in neural information processing systems 30 (2017).
  16. Folk psychological attributions of consciousness to large language models.
  17. The evolution of cooperation in infinitely repeated games: Experimental evidence. American Economic Review 101, 1 (2011), 411–429.
  18. De Villiers, J. The interface of language and theory of mind. Lingua 117, 11 (2007), 1858–1878.
  19. Negotiating with other minds: the role of recursive theory of mind in negotiation with incomplete information. Autonomous Agents and Multi-Agent Systems 31 (2017), 250–287.
  20. Higher-order theory of mind is especially useful in unpredictable negotiations. Autonomous Agents and Multi-Agent Systems 36, 2 (2022), 30.
  21. Dennett, D. C. The intentional stance. MIT press, 1989.
  22. Francois-Lovens, P. “without these conversations with the eliza chatbot, my husband would still be here”. La Libre.
  23. Evolution and cooperation in noisy repeated games. The American Economic Review 80, 2 (1990), 274–279.
  24. Gabriel, I. Artificial intelligence, values, and alignment. Minds and machines 30, 3 (2020), 411–437.
  25. Fairness considerations: increasing understanding of intentionality during adolescence. Journal of experimental child psychology 104, 4 (2009), 398–409.
  26. Incomplete contracting and ai alignment. In Proceedings of the 2019 AAAI/ACM Conference on AI, Ethics, and Society (2019), pp. 417–422.
  27. Cooperative inverse reinforcement learning. Advances in neural information processing systems 29 (2016).
  28. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: a critical review. Cognitive neuropsychiatry 10, 4 (2005), 249–286.
  29. Mentalizing about emotion and its relationship to empathy. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 3, 3 (2008), 204–217.
  30. Theory-of-mind deficits and causal attributions. British journal of Psychology 89, 2 (1998), 191–204.
  31. Knobe, J. Theory of mind and moral cognition: Exploring the connections. Trends in cognitive sciences 9, 8 (2005), 357–359.
  32. Kosinski, M. Theory of mind may have spontaneously emerged in large language models. arXiv preprint arXiv:2302.02083 (2023).
  33. Specification gaming: the flip side of ai ingenuity, 21 April 2020.
  34. Rational cooperation in the finitely repeated prisoners’ dilemma. Journal of Economic theory 27, 2 (1982), 245–252.
  35. Theory of mind and emotion understanding predict moral development in early childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 28, 4 (2010), 871–889.
  36. Learning maximum absolute meaning through reasoning about speaker intentions. Language Learning 71, 2 (2021), 326–368.
  37. Littler, K. Cognitive and affective processes associated with moral reasoning, and their relationship with behaviour in typical development.
  38. To lie or not to lie? the influence of parenting and theory-of-mind understanding on three-year-old children’s honesty. Journal of Moral Education 44, 2 (2015), 198–212.
  39. Malle, B. F. How the mind explains behavior. Folk explanation, Meaning and social interaction. Massachusetts: MIT-Press (2004).
  40. Reliability and validity of the awareness of social inference test (tasit): a clinical test of social perception. Disability and rehabilitation 28, 24 (2006), 1529–1542.
  41. How is human cooperation different? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 365, 1553 (2010), 2663–2674.
  42. Should robots be obedient? arXiv preprint arXiv:1705.09990 (2017).
  43. Generative agents: Interactive simulacra of human behavior. In Proceedings of the 36th Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software and Technology (2023), pp. 1–22.
  44. Ai deception: A survey of examples, risks, and potential solutions. arXiv preprint arXiv:2308.14752 (2023).
  45. Discovering language model behaviors with model-written evaluations. arXiv preprint arXiv:2212.09251 (2022).
  46. Orbital prefrontal cortex volume correlates with social cognitive competence. Neuropsychologia 48, 12 (2010), 3554–3562.
  47. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and brain sciences 1, 4 (1978), 515–526.
  48. Theory of mind ability and cooperation. Manuscript, Univ. California, Irvine (2017).
  49. The development of interpersonal strategy: Autism, theory-of-mind, cooperation and fairness. Journal of economic psychology 27, 1 (2006), 73–97.
  50. Schwitzgebel, E. Ai systems must not confuse users about their sentience or moral status. Patterns 4, 8 (2023).
  51. Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain 132, 3 (2009), 617–627.
  52. Clever hans or neural theory of mind? stress testing social reasoning in large language models. arXiv preprint arXiv:2305.14763 (2023).
  53. Shevlin, H. Uncanny believers: Chatbots, beliefs, and folk psychology. Unpublished manuscript (2021).
  54. Why be nice? psychological constraints on the evolution of cooperation. Trends in cognitive sciences 8, 2 (2004), 60–65.
  55. Perspective-taking and memory capacity predict social network size. Social Networks 29, 1 (2007), 93–104.
  56. How children tell a lie from a joke: The role of second-order mental state attributions. British journal of developmental psychology 13, 2 (1995), 191–204.
  57. Bullying and ‘theory of mind’: A critique of the ‘social skills deficit’view of anti-social behaviour. Social development 8, 1 (1999), 117–127.
  58. Social cognition and bullying: Social inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British journal of developmental psychology 17, 3 (1999), 435–450.
  59. Theory of mind enhances preference for fairness. Journal of experimental child psychology 105, 1-2 (2010), 130–137.
  60. Ullman, T. Large language models fail on trivial alterations to theory-of-mind tasks. arXiv preprint arXiv:2302.08399 (2023).
  61. Deficiencies in theory of mind in patients with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder: A systematic review of secondary literature. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 120 (2021), 249–261.
  62. A survey on large language model based autonomous agents, 2023.
  63. Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. science 311, 5765 (2006), 1301–1303.
  64. Ethical and social risks of harm from language models. arXiv preprint arXiv:2112.04359 (2021).
  65. Young children’s reasoning about beliefs. Cognition 30, 3 (1988), 239–277.
  66. Including deontic reasoning as fundamental to theory of mind. Human Development 51, 2 (2008), 105–135.
  67. A survey of large language models. arXiv preprint arXiv:2303.18223 (2023).
Citations (5)

Summary

  • The paper demonstrates that LLM theory of mind can refine goal specification by interpreting ambiguous human intentions, leading to better alignment with user values.
  • The paper shows that using ToM improves conversational adaptation, enabling LLMs to adjust tone and content while highlighting risks like manipulation and discrimination.
  • The paper highlights that incorporating ToM in LLMs can support collective ethical judgments and social cooperation, though it may also introduce competitive biases and misuse.

LLM Theory of Mind and Alignment: Opportunities and Risks

Introduction

The paper "LLM Theory of Mind and Alignment: Opportunities and Risks" explores the interaction between LLMs, theory of mind (ToM), and their alignment with human values. ToM refers to the ability to infer the mental and emotional states of oneself and others, a critical component of human social intelligence. The paper examines whether LLMs possess ToM and how this capability could potentially enhance alignment with human values in both individual and group interactions.

Individual Level Implications

Goal Specification

The paper discusses the potential of LLMs to refine goal specification by interpreting ambiguous human goals and aligning them more closely with user intentions. This could mitigate 'misspecification' issues where systems meet objectives without fulfilling user intentions. However, inaccuracies in ToM could lead to misinterpretation of user goals, especially in high-stakes areas like finance or healthcare. Additionally, the ethical implications arise when an LLM autonomously curtails a user's inappropriate goals.

Conversational Adaptation

LLMs leveraging ToM can enhance conversational adaptation by adjusting tone, register, and content based on inferred user states. This adaptability might improve user experiences and comprehension of information. Nevertheless, risks such as informational inequality and discrimination can emerge when LLMs tailor responses differently based on perceived user knowledge levels. The paper highlights concerns about manipulation and deception, emphasizing the need for empirical validation of adaptive strategies in LLM outputs.

Empathy and Anthropomorphism

Empathetic interactions facilitated by LLM ToM may improve user support in contexts like therapy or education. However, this poses risks of promoting over-reliance, leading to pathological relationships with AI systems. Anthropomorphism—the attribution of human-like qualities to LLMs—can create unrealistic user expectations and mislead users concerning the AI's capabilities. The paper urges further research to assess the effect of anthropomorphism on user behavior, privacy, and ethical concerns.

Group Level Implications

Collective Alignment

LLM ToM can aid collective alignment by interpreting societal-level ethical principles during model fine-tuning and resolving conflicts in multi-party scenarios. High-order intentionality—the ability to chain multiple mental states—enhanced by ToM might allow LLMs to arbitrate complex social interactions. Nonetheless, these capacities must be carefully managed to avoid excessive influence over human affairs and to account for disparities in access to advanced LLMs.

Cooperation and Competition

The paper juxtaposes ToM's role in fostering cooperation against its potential to engender competitive, antisocial behaviors. While ToM might drive LLMs towards prosocial group dynamics, it could also provide competitive advantages that exacerbate social inequalities. LLMs' capabilities exceeding human cognitive limits pose additional challenges, demanding consideration of ethics surrounding AI competition.

Moral Judgement

Incorporating ToM into moral reasoning may align LLM judgments with human norms. The paper suggests leveraging LLMs' ability to predict user states to guide ethical AI decision-making. It explores potential bias in ToM inferences influenced by moral contexts and advocates for setting objective standards in LLM judgments to prevent biased conclusions.

Conclusion

The paper presents a comprehensive exploration of LLM ToM's role in aligning AI systems with human values, highlighting both opportunities and risks. Successful ToM inferences could advance user interaction, goal achievement, and collective alignment while posing challenges when manipulated by unethical actors or leading to pathological user-AI relationships. Continued research should focus on mitigating potential misuse, fostering positive ethical AI applications, and balancing short-term satisfaction with long-term goals.

List To Do Tasks Checklist Streamline Icon: https://streamlinehq.com

Collections

Sign up for free to add this paper to one or more collections.

Authors (1)

X Twitter Logo Streamline Icon: https://streamlinehq.com

Tweets